Adaptive Parsons problems, and the role of SES and Gesture in learning computing: ICER 2018 Preview
August 10, 2018 at 7:00 am 7 comments
Next week is the 2018 International Computing Education Research Conference in Espoo, Finland. The proceedings are (as of this writing) available here: https://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=3230977. Our group has three papers in the 28 accepted this year.
“Evaluating the efficiency and effectiveness of adaptive Parsons problems” by Barbara Ericson, Jim Foley, and Jochen (“Jeff”) Rick
These are the final studies from Barb Ericson’s dissertation (I blogged about her defense here). In her experiment, she compared four conditions: Students learning through writing code, through fixing code, through solving Parsons problems, and through solving her new adaptive Parsons problems. She had a control group this time (different from her Koli Calling paper) that did turtle graphics between the pre-test and post-test, so that she could be sure that there wasn’t just a testing effect of pre-test followed by a post-test. The bottom line was basically what she predicted: Learning did occur, with no significant difference between treatment groups, but the Parsons problems groups took less time. Our ebooks now include some of her adaptive Parsons problems, so she can compare performance across many students on adaptive and non-adaptive forms of the same problem. She finds that students solve the problems more and with fewer trials on the adaptive problems. So, adaptive Parsons problems lead to the same amount of learning, in less time, with fewer failures. (Failures matter, since self-efficacy is a big deal in computer science education.)
“Socioeconomic status and Computer science achievement: Spatial ability as a mediating variable in a novel model of understanding” by Miranda Parker, Amber Solomon, Brianna Pritchett, David Illingworth, Lauren Margulieux, and Mark Guzdial
(Link to last version I reviewed.)
This study is a response to the paper Steve Cooper presented at ICER 2015 (see blog post here), where they found that spatial reasoning training erased performance differences between higher and lower socioeconomic status (SES) students, while the comparison class had higher-SES students performing better than lower-SES students. Miranda and Amber wanted to test this relationship at a larger scale.
Why should wealthier students do better in CS? The most common reason I’ve heard is that wealthier students have more opportunities to study CS — they have greater access. Sometimes that’s called preparatory privilege.
Miranda and Amber and their team wanted to test whether access is really the right intermediate variable. They gave students at two different Universities four tests:
- Part of Miranda’s SCS1 to measure performance in CS.
- A standardized test of SES.
- A test of spatial reasoning.
- A survey about the amount of access they had to CS education, e.g., formal classes, code clubs, summer camps, etc.
David and Lauren did the factor analysis and structural equation modeling to compare two hypotheses: Does higher SES lead to greater access which leads to greater success in CS, or does higher SES lead to higher spatial reasoning which leads to greater success in CS? Neither hypothesis accounted for a significant amount of the differences in CS performance, but the spatial reasoning model did better than the access model.
There are some significant limitations of this study. The biggest is that they gathered data at universities. A lot of SES variance just disappears when you look at college students — they tend to be wealthier than average.
Still, the result is important for challenging the prevailing assumption about why wealthier kids do better in CS. More, spatial reasoning is an interesting variable because it’s rather inexpensively taught. It’s expensive to prepare CS teachers and get them into all schools. Steve showed that we can teach spatial reasoning within an existing CS class and reduce SES differences.
“Applying a Gesture Taxonomy to Introductory Computing Concepts” by Amber Solomon, Betsy DiSalvo, Mark Guzdial, and Ben Shapiro
We were a bit surprised (quite pleasantly!) that this paper got into ICER. I love the paper, but it’s different from most ICER papers.
Amber is interested in the role that gestures play in teaching CS. She started this paper from a taxonomy of gestures seen in other STEM classes. She observed a CS classroom and used her observations to provide concrete examples of the gestures seen in other kinds of classes. This isn’t a report of empirical findings. This is a report of using a lens borrowed from another field to look at CS learning and teaching in a new way.
My favorite part of of this paper is when Amber points out what parts of CS gestures don’t really fit in the taxonomy. It’s one thing to point to lines of code – that’s relatively concrete. It’s another thing to “point” to reference data, e.g., when explaining a sort and you gesture at the two elements you’re comparing or swapping. What exactly/concretely are we pointing at? Arrays are neither horizontal nor vertical — that distinction doesn’t really exist in memory. Arrays have no physical representation, but we act (usually) as if they’re laid out horizontally in front of us. What assumptions are we making in order to use gestures in our teaching? And what if students don’t share in those assumptions?
Entry filed under: Uncategorized. Tags: assessment, computing education research, ICER, Parsons Problems, SES, spatial reasoning.
1.
What do I mean by Computing Education Research? The Computer Science Perspective | Computing Education Research Blog | November 12, 2018 at 8:01 am
[…] work, we have been studying the role of spatial reasoning and gesture in learning to program (see summaries of our ICER 2018 papers). We don’t know why spatial reasoning might be playing a role in learning to program. Maybe […]
2.
The problem with sorting students into CS classes: We don’t know how, and we may institutionalize inequity | Computing Education Research Blog | April 1, 2019 at 7:01 am
[…] that wealth (more specifically, socioeconomic status (SES)) leads to success in computing (see this paper), and we have a literature review identifying other privileges that likely lead to success in CS […]
3.
Learning to build machine learning applications without code as an example of computing education research | Computing Education Research Blog | July 8, 2019 at 2:01 am
[…] notional machine harder to teach. There is no code to point at when explaining an algorithm (see Amber Solomon’s work on the role of gestures in teaching CS). Maybe you wouldn’t explain an algorithm. Maybe instead you’d point at examples […]
4.
An Analysis of Supports and Barriers to Offering Computer Science in Georgia Public High Schools: Miranda Parker’s Defense | Computing Education Research Blog | October 7, 2019 at 7:01 am
[…] validated measure of CS1 knowledge, her study of teacher-student differences in using ebooks, and her work exploring the role of spatial reasoning to relate SES and CS performance (work that was part of her dissertation study). I’m looking […]
5.
Why don’t high schools teach CS: It’s the lack of teachers, but it’s way more than that (Miranda Parker's dissertation) | Computing Education Research Blog | December 16, 2019 at 8:00 am
[…] of CS1 knowledge, the study she did of teacher-student differences in using ebooks, and her work exploring the role of spatial reasoning to relate SES and CS performance (work that was part of her dissertation […]
6.
How do teachers teach recursion with embodiment, and why won’t students trace their programs: ICLS 2020 Preview | Computing Education Research Blog | June 15, 2020 at 7:00 am
[…] science learning to analyze the gestures she saw in a high school computer science classroom (see link here). Last summer at ITiCSE, she published a paper on how making CS visible in the classroom (through […]
7.
Embodiment in CS Learning: How Space, Metaphor, Gesture, and Sketching Support Student Learning: Amber Solomon’s defense | Computing Education Research Blog | April 12, 2021 at 9:00 am
[…] status (SES) predicts CS performance. In general, rich kids do better in CS than poor kids. Why? They compared two different models for why SES predicted performance on a standardized CS test. One model suggested that higher SES led to greater access to CS education. Rich kids got to take […]